Maladaptive Cognitive Bias in the New Normal Period: An Analyses from a Behavioural Science Perspective in the Time of Covid-19
Abstract
Keywords
DOI: 10.22146/buletinpsikologi.60763
References
Advani, S. D., Yarrington, M. E., Smith, B. A., Anderson, D. J., & Sexton, D. J. (2020). Are we forgetting the “universal” in universal masking? Current challenges and future solutions. Infection Control and Hospital Epidemiology, 1–2. doi: 10.1017/ice. 2020.333
Arkes, H. R. (1991). Costs and benefits of judgment errors: Implications for debiasing. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 486–498. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.486
Baron, J. (2000). Thinking and deciding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bavel, J. J. V., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S. et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support Covid-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav, 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
Bickel, W. K., Moody, L., & Higgins, S. T. (2016). Some current dimensions of the behavioural economics of health-related behaviour change. Preventive medicine, 92, 16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
Bottemanne, H., Morlaàs, O., Fossati, P., & Schmidt, L. (2020). Does the coronavirus epidemic take advantage of human optimism bias? Frontiers in Psychology, 11(August), 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02001
Burns, Z., Chiu, A., & Wu, G. (2010). Overweighting of small probabilities. In J. J. Cochran (Ed.), Wiley encyclopedia of operations research and management science (pp.1-8). New York: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470400531.eorms0634
Centola, D. (2011) An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of health behaviour. Science, 334(6060), 1269–1272. doi: 10.1126/science.1207055
Cialdini, R. B. & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu Rev Psychol. 55(44), 591–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015
Fleming, S. M., Thomas, C. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Overcoming status quo bias in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(13), 6005–6009. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910380107
Gallagher, K. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behaviour: A meta-analytic review. Annals of Behavioural Medicine, 43(1), 101-116. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9308-7
gandhi, m., & rutherford, g. w. (2020). facial masking for covid-19 — potential for “variolation” as we Await a Vaccine. New England Journal of Medicine, 383(18), e101. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp2026913
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research 35, 472–482. doi: 10.1086/586910
Halpern, S. D., Truog, R. D., & Miller, F. G. (2020). Cognitive bias and public health policy during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA, 324(4), 337. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.11623
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81–91. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81
Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. The Evolution of Cognitive Bias, 724-746.
Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2015). The evolution of cognitive bias. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 724–746.
Hume, S., John, P., Sanders, M., & Stockdale, E. (2020). Nudge in the time of Coronavirus: The persistence of behavioural messages during crisis. SSRN Electronic Journal. doi: 10.2139/ssrn
Jordan, J., Yoeli, E., & Rand, D. (2020). Don’t get it or don’t spread it? Comparing self-interested versus pro-socially framed Covid-19 prevention messaging. Working paper, https://psyarxiv.com/yuq7
Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioural economics. American Economic Review, 93, 1449–1475. doi: 10.1257/000282803322655392
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. doi: 10.2307/1914185
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychological Review, 103, 582-591. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.582
Kai, D., Goldstein, G. P., Morgunov, A., Nangalia, V., & Rotkirch, A. (2020). Universal masking is urgent in the COVID-19 Pandemic: SEIR and agent based models, empirical validation, policy recommendations. ArXiv.
Loewenstein, G., Brennan, T., & Volpp, K. G. (2007). Asymmetric paternalism
to improve health behaviours. Journal of the American Medical Association, 298(20), 2415-2417. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.202415
Loewenstein, G., John, L. K., & Volpp, K.G. (2012). Using decision errors to help people help themselves. In E. Shafir (Ed.), The behavioural foundations of public policy (pp.361-379). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Lunn, P. D., Timmons, S., Barjaková, M., Belton, C. A., Julienne, H., & Lavin, C. (2020). Motivating social distancing during the Covid-19 Pandemic: An online experiment. ESRI Working Paper, 658.
Marteau, T. M., Ogilvie, D., Roland, M., Suhrcke, M., &; Kelly, M. P. (2011). Judging nudging: Can nudging improve population health? BMJ,342(3), 228. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d228
McDermott, R., Fowler, J. H., & Smirnov, O. (2008). On the evolutionary origin of prospect theory preferences. Journal of Politics, 70(2), 335–350. doi: 10.1017/S0022381608080341
Nettle, D. (2004). Adaptive illusions: optimism, control, and human rationality. In Emotion, Evolution, and Rationality (pp. 193–208). doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528975.003.0010
O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (1999). Doing it now or later. American Economic Review, 89(1), 103-124. doi: 10.1257/aer.89.1.103
Samuelson, W., & Zeckhauser, R. (1988). Status quo bias in decision making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1(1), 7–59. doi: 10.1007/BF00055564
Saposnik, G., Redelmeier, D., Ruff, C. C., & Tobler, P. N. (2016). Cognitive biases associated with medical decisions: a systematic review. BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making, 16(1), 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12911-016-0377-1
Sasaki, S., Kurokawa, H., & Ohtake, F. (2020). Short-term responses to nudge-based messages for preventing the spread of Covid-19 infection: Intention, behaviour, and life satisfaction. Discussion Papers in Economics and Business No. 20-11, Osaka University, Graduate School of Economics. Retrieved from https://econpapers.repec.org/paper/oskwpaper/2011.htm
Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Current Biology, 21(23), R941-R945. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030
Smith, M. (2020). International COVID-19 tracker update: 2 September. Retrieved November 16, 2020, from YouGov Covid-19 Tracker website: https://today.yougov.com/topics/health/articles-reports/2020/09/02/international-covid-19-tracker-update-2-september
Soofi, M., Najafi, F., & Karami-Matin, B. (2020). Using insights from behavioural economics to mitigate the spread of Covid-19. Applied Health Economics and Health Policy, 18, 345-350. doi: 10.1007/s40258-020-00595-4
Thaler, R. H. (2016). Behavioural economics: Past, present, and future. American Economic Review, 106(7), 1577-600. doi: 10.1257/aer.106.7.1577
Thaler, R. H. (2018). From cashews to nudges: The evolution of behavioural economics. American Economic Review, 108(6), 1265-1287. doi: 10.1257/aer.108.6.1265
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. doi: 10.1126/science.7455683
van der Pligt, J., Otten, W., Richard, R., & van der Velde, F. (1993). Perceived risk of AIDS: unrealistic optimism and self-protective action. In J. B. Pryor, & G. D. Reeder (Eds.). The social psychology of HIV-infection (pp.39-58). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
van der Pol, M., Hennessy, D., & Manns, B. (2017). The role of time and risk preferences in adherence to physician advice on health behaviour change. The European Journal of Health Economics, 18(3), 373-386. doi: 10.1007/s10198-016-0800-7
Weber, B. J., & Chapman, G. B. (2005). Playing for peanuts: Why is risk seeking more common for low-stakes gambles? Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 97(1), 31-46. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.03.001
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrelistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 806-820. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806
Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 10(5), 481-500. doi: 10.1007/BF00846146
Weinstein, N. D., Marcus, S. E., & Moser, R. P. (2005). Smokers’ unrealistic optimism about their risk. Tobacco Control, 14(1), 55-59. doi: 10.1136/tc.2004.008375
World Health Organization (2020). WHO announces Covid-19 outbreak a pandemic. Retrieved September 23, 2020, from:https://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-emergencies/coronavirus-Covid-19/news/news/2020/3/who-announces-Covid-19-outbreak-a-pandemicRefbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
Copyright (c) 2020 Buletin Psikologi
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.